Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve

|

October 30, 2023

Unlock the secrets of memory retention with the Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve. Explore its implications for educators and learn strategies to improve learning.

Course Enquiry
Copy citation

Benjamin, Z (2023, October 30). Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve. Retrieved from https://www.structural-learning.com/post/ebbinghaus-forgetting-curve

What is the Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve?

The Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve, a seminal construct in the psychology of memory, was formulated by Hermann Ebbinghaus in the waning years of the 19th century.

This curve doesn't just delineate how we forget; it's an existential map of our cognitive landscape. Picture this: Within the first two hours post-learning, your memory retention plummets in an almost catastrophic fashion.

It's not a linear descent but an exponential one. Yet, the curve stabilizes over the ensuing days, settling into a more gradual rate of decline. This isn't mere academic jargon; it's a call to arms for educators.

Recognizing the mechanics of forgetting can be the first step in crafting pedagogical strategies that not only stick but also transform.

This article aims to be your navigational chart through the labyrinthine intricacies of the Forgetting Curve. We'll delve into its theoretical underpinnings, offer pragmatic advice for its application in the classroom, and illuminate its relevance in contemporary educational settings.

Key Insights

  • "Memory is the residue of thought," posits cognitive psychologist Daniel Willingham.
  • The Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve is an exponential model of how information is lost over time if not revisited.
  • The curve is most steep within the first two hours after learning, making immediate review crucial.

 

What does the Curve Teach us About Forgetting?

The forgetting curve shows that we forget the majority of new information soon after it is initially learnt. Ebbinghaus defined forgetting as an ability to recall information in the absence of any cues.

Our ability to recognise new information does not follow the same pattern; being presented with cues or multiple-choice options increases the accuracy of our memory.

While the curve describes a general trend to forget new information when there is no attempt to retain it, there will be individual variations in the shape of the curve. 

New information that is of significant value, is related to a major event, or offers a surprising contradiction to previously learnt material, is less likely to be forgotten at such a rapid rate.

What Factors Influence the Decline of Memory?

Time has the greatest impact on the decline of memory; memory retention over time is very poor in the absence of any attempt to retain the new information.  The total amount of information that is forgotten increases with time, but the majority of this happens soon after learning has occurred.  

The quality of learning also has a significant impact on whether the new material will be resistant to the steep decline in memory observed by Ebbinghaus. Information that is fully understood or deeply processed is likely to be forgotten less quickly.

Similarly, if the new information is of personal significance or has a practical application, it is more likely to be remembered well, partly because the person is more motivated to encode it effectively into their long-term memory.

When new information is similar or related to prior learning, it can impact the decline in memory in both directions.

Retention increases if new knowledge is assimilated into a pre-existing schema of related information in the long-term memory because the prior learning offers an abundance of cues for the new information.

However, when information is similar but unrelated to something that has been previously learnt it can increase the decline in memory for the new information (proactive interference) or the previously learnt information (retroactive interference).

Both types of interference can be seen when learning a new foreign language; words from a previously learnt foreign language may be forgotten when the new language is learnt (retroactive) or it can be more difficult to learn new vocabulary if it is too similar to the previously learnt vocabulary.

There will be individual differences in memory strength, even for nonsense syllables (three-letter ‘words’ that have no meaning). Some of the reasons for this variation include:

  • Age: the decline in memory increases with age.
  • Cognitive ability: the decline in memory decreases with cognitive ability.
  • Levels of stress and anxiety: moderate levels improve memory while high levels will impair it.
  • Sleep: a lack of sleep causes a faster decline in memory.
  • Personal significance: increased motivation associated with personal significance improves memory retention.

 

Ebbinghaus-forgetting-curve
Ebbinghaus-forgetting-curve

3 Ways to Challenge the Forgetting Curve

The Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve describes the decline in memory when there is no attempt to retain the new information. Any strategy that is designed to increase retention will challenge the decline in memory and flatten the forgetting curve.  

Repeated Retrieval Practice

The most effective way to challenge the forgetting curve is through repeated retrieval practice. The first attempt to retrieve the new information should be soon after it was originally learnt, with subsequent retrievals becoming increasingly spread out over longer time periods (spaced repetition).

Each successful retrieval increases the number of cues associated with the information, which flattens the forgetting curve and makes it possible to extend the length of time between future retrievals.

Forgetting curve and retrieval practice
Forgetting curve and retrieval practice

Techniques to Improve Memory

Developing techniques to improve memory will reduce the rate of forgetting, even in the absence of any further attempts to retain the new information.

Mnemonic devices focus on encoding new information in a way that will make it easier to retrieve it in the future. Using acronyms can be very successful when it is necessary to learn the order of a list of words:

Never eat shredded wheat: the clockwise order of the compass points (North, East, South, West).

Richard of York gave battle in vain: the order of colours in a rainbow.

Another popular technique is creating a memory palace. This involves developing a mental image of a real or imaginary location, often a house or palace, that has vivid and distinct locations throughout it.

When trying to memorise a list of words, images, or facts, each one is associated with one of the locations in a vivid or meaningful way. Visualising the memory palace then acts as a prompt to remember the new material.

 

Memory Prompts During Learning

Having an awareness of memory prompts and incorporating them into the learning process will make it easier to recall the newly learnt information at a later date and slow down the steep decline in memory.

Linking new information to prior learning is one of the most effective ways to achieve this; incorporating the new material into a pre-existing schema will allow it to benefit from all of the existing memory prompts and cues that are already in place.

Alternatively, consider using physical cues or the environment as a memory prompt. For example, writing key terms in a different colour pen, in capital letters, or in a certain position on a piece of paper can act as a memory prompt during subsequent recall attempts.

It can also help to learn new material in the same, or similar, environment to where it will need to be recalled; revising in an exam hall in silence would be more effective than revising in a bedroom with music.

 

Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve and retrieval
Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve and retrieval

Implications for Teachers

Understanding how students forget can allow teachers to make changes to their practice to challenge the Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve and increase their students’ memory retention. The following approaches are recommended to teachers in response to Ebbinghaus’ research:

1. Regular Retrieval Practice

Repeated exposure to information strengthens memory retention and flattens the forgetting curve. Asking students to retrieve information forces them to revisit the information, even if they recall it incorrectly, due to the corrective feedback that they receive.

The first retrieval practice should be soon after the new material has been learnt, preferably the following day or in the next lesson. The period of time between each subsequent retrieval should be longer than the previous one. Using spaced intervals are also recommended in Rosenshine’s principles of instruction:

  • Begin each lesson with a review of previous learning
  • Have weekly and monthly reviews of previous learning

It is better to interleave two or more topics together during retrieval practice; this allows the material to be revisited more often and spaced practice to be spread out over time.

2. Review Schemes of Work

Schemes of work should be reviewed to ensure they include regular opportunities for spaced retrieval practice and that topics are arranged by increasing level of difficulty to build on prior learning.

Curriculum plans should be designed to promote mastery by using scaffolding and dividing complex information into smaller and more manageable chunks.

By reducing the volume of new material that students need to learn in each part of the lesson, they are more likely to encode it effectively into their long-term memories.

Lesson plans should make the links to prior learning explicit to students as this will help them assimilate the new information into a pre-existing schema.

It is also helpful to list the keywords and related keywords for each topic as this will help students to reorganise the new information, provide more cues to aid recall, and allow them to make links between related topics.

Promote Metacognition

Encourage students to reflect on what they have learned, but also how they learned it. This will help students to understand which strategies are most effective at improving memory retention.

This is particularly important after a test or assessment, and part of the teacher’s feedback should be focussed on the effectiveness of the revision strategies and processes that the student used.

 

Hermann Ebbinghaus
Hermann Ebbinghaus

Implications for Students 

Being able to accurately recall information gives students a distinct advantage in our current educational system and is a precursor to being able to effectively manipulate and evaluate that information.

Practising active recall strategies and using memory-enhancing techniques as often as possible will challenge the decline in memory that occurs in the absence of retrieval attempts and memory retention strategies.

Active recall requires students to access information from their long-term memory in the absence of any memory cues or prompts.

A brain dump is one of the most simple and effective ways to achieve this. It involves writing down everything the student can remember about a given topic within a specified time frame.

There are no restrictions or demands about what information can be recalled, which means that students will often also benefit from hearing what their peers have been able to recall. Answering practice questions, defining or generating a list of keywords, or completing an assessment are other useful examples of active recall.

Activities that involve passive recall are much less effective at improving memory. These include:

  • Re-reading notes
  • Highlighting text
  • Answering multiple-choice questions

Summarising a page of text or using flashcards can be classified as being either active or passive recall depending on how each task is approached.

Summarising a page of text is a passive recall activity if the page of text is available throughout the task. However, it becomes an active recall activity if the student reads the text, puts it away, and then writes a summary from memory.

The latter approach should be used for answering practice questions; always read the text and hide it before attempting to answer a question about it.

Using flashcards to aid revision by reading the question and then turning over to ‘confirm’ you know what the answer was involves passive recall at best. However, writing down the answer or answering the question out loud before turning over the card to check the answer would be an example of active recall.

Mnemonic techniques are an effective way to boost memory, especially when it is necessary to remember a list of words in order. Students may also benefit from using dual coding to memorise key words or definitions; this involves pairing the new material with a particularly vivid image to make it more memorable.

 

Understanding Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve
Understanding Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve

Background Information and Further Reading

Hermann Ebbinghaus was a German psychologist who conducted pioneering research on memory and learning in the late 19th century.

He conducted systematic research on himself by memorising series of nonsense syllables (meaningless combinations of three letters) and counting how many he could remember after varying intervals of time.

He discovered that the majority of forgetting happened in the first few hours and that the rate of forgetting decreased over time. 

This trend was presented using the graphical representation of a forgetting curve.  Ebbinghaus’ work was an example of groundbreaking experimental psychology at the time due to the empirical approach he adopted to the study of memory. 

A disadvantage of his approach is that it oversimplifies the human brain and the nature of memory loss by only testing the memorisation of nonsense syllables.  It was difficult to know whether the results could be applied to more natural learning environments.

Subsequent research has supported the concept of an exponential forgetting curve and the conclusions that can be drawn from Ebbinghaus’ research to challenge the decline in memory have been shown to effectively improve memory in real-life settings. To learn more about these studies and the Ebbinghaus forgetting curve, please use the links below.

  1. Murre, J., & Dros, J. (2015). Replication and Analysis of Ebbinghaus’ Forgetting Curve. PLoS ONE, 10.
  2. Roe, D. G., Kim, S., Choi, Y. Y., Woo, H., Kang, M., Song, Y., Ahn, J., Lee, Y., & Cho, J. (2021). Biologically Plausible Artificial Synaptic Array: Replicating Ebbinghaus’ Memory Curve with Selective Attention. Advanced Materials, 33.
  3. Jaber, M., & Bonney, M. (1996). Production breaks and the learning curve: The forgetting phenomenon. Applied Mathematical Modelling, 20, 162-169. 
  4. Miller, R. R. (2021). Failures of memory and the fate of forgotten memories. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 181.
  5. Hewitt, D., Sprague, K., Yearout, R., Lisnerski, D., & Sparks, C. (1992). The effects of unequal relearning rates on estimating forgetting parameters associated with performance curves. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 10, 217-224.

 

Step 1/6
Your free resource

Enhance outcomes across your school

Download an overview of our classroom toolkit.

Step 2/6
Contact Details

We'll send it over now.

Please fill in the details so we can send over the resources.

Step 3/6
School Type

What type of school are you?

We'll get you the right resource

Step 4/6
CPD

Is your school involved in any staff development projects?

Are your colleagues running any research projects or courses?

Step 5/6
Priorities

Do you have any immediate school priorities?

Please check the ones that apply.

Step 6/6
Confirmation

Download your resource

Thanks for taking the time to complete this form, submit the form to get the tool.

Previous
Next step
Thanks, submission has been recieved.

Click below to download.
Download
Oops! Something went wrong while submitting the form

Psychology

What is the Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve?

The Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve, a seminal construct in the psychology of memory, was formulated by Hermann Ebbinghaus in the waning years of the 19th century.

This curve doesn't just delineate how we forget; it's an existential map of our cognitive landscape. Picture this: Within the first two hours post-learning, your memory retention plummets in an almost catastrophic fashion.

It's not a linear descent but an exponential one. Yet, the curve stabilizes over the ensuing days, settling into a more gradual rate of decline. This isn't mere academic jargon; it's a call to arms for educators.

Recognizing the mechanics of forgetting can be the first step in crafting pedagogical strategies that not only stick but also transform.

This article aims to be your navigational chart through the labyrinthine intricacies of the Forgetting Curve. We'll delve into its theoretical underpinnings, offer pragmatic advice for its application in the classroom, and illuminate its relevance in contemporary educational settings.

Key Insights

  • "Memory is the residue of thought," posits cognitive psychologist Daniel Willingham.
  • The Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve is an exponential model of how information is lost over time if not revisited.
  • The curve is most steep within the first two hours after learning, making immediate review crucial.

 

What does the Curve Teach us About Forgetting?

The forgetting curve shows that we forget the majority of new information soon after it is initially learnt. Ebbinghaus defined forgetting as an ability to recall information in the absence of any cues.

Our ability to recognise new information does not follow the same pattern; being presented with cues or multiple-choice options increases the accuracy of our memory.

While the curve describes a general trend to forget new information when there is no attempt to retain it, there will be individual variations in the shape of the curve. 

New information that is of significant value, is related to a major event, or offers a surprising contradiction to previously learnt material, is less likely to be forgotten at such a rapid rate.

What Factors Influence the Decline of Memory?

Time has the greatest impact on the decline of memory; memory retention over time is very poor in the absence of any attempt to retain the new information.  The total amount of information that is forgotten increases with time, but the majority of this happens soon after learning has occurred.  

The quality of learning also has a significant impact on whether the new material will be resistant to the steep decline in memory observed by Ebbinghaus. Information that is fully understood or deeply processed is likely to be forgotten less quickly.

Similarly, if the new information is of personal significance or has a practical application, it is more likely to be remembered well, partly because the person is more motivated to encode it effectively into their long-term memory.

When new information is similar or related to prior learning, it can impact the decline in memory in both directions.

Retention increases if new knowledge is assimilated into a pre-existing schema of related information in the long-term memory because the prior learning offers an abundance of cues for the new information.

However, when information is similar but unrelated to something that has been previously learnt it can increase the decline in memory for the new information (proactive interference) or the previously learnt information (retroactive interference).

Both types of interference can be seen when learning a new foreign language; words from a previously learnt foreign language may be forgotten when the new language is learnt (retroactive) or it can be more difficult to learn new vocabulary if it is too similar to the previously learnt vocabulary.

There will be individual differences in memory strength, even for nonsense syllables (three-letter ‘words’ that have no meaning). Some of the reasons for this variation include:

  • Age: the decline in memory increases with age.
  • Cognitive ability: the decline in memory decreases with cognitive ability.
  • Levels of stress and anxiety: moderate levels improve memory while high levels will impair it.
  • Sleep: a lack of sleep causes a faster decline in memory.
  • Personal significance: increased motivation associated with personal significance improves memory retention.

 

Ebbinghaus-forgetting-curve
Ebbinghaus-forgetting-curve

3 Ways to Challenge the Forgetting Curve

The Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve describes the decline in memory when there is no attempt to retain the new information. Any strategy that is designed to increase retention will challenge the decline in memory and flatten the forgetting curve.  

Repeated Retrieval Practice

The most effective way to challenge the forgetting curve is through repeated retrieval practice. The first attempt to retrieve the new information should be soon after it was originally learnt, with subsequent retrievals becoming increasingly spread out over longer time periods (spaced repetition).

Each successful retrieval increases the number of cues associated with the information, which flattens the forgetting curve and makes it possible to extend the length of time between future retrievals.

Forgetting curve and retrieval practice
Forgetting curve and retrieval practice

Techniques to Improve Memory

Developing techniques to improve memory will reduce the rate of forgetting, even in the absence of any further attempts to retain the new information.

Mnemonic devices focus on encoding new information in a way that will make it easier to retrieve it in the future. Using acronyms can be very successful when it is necessary to learn the order of a list of words:

Never eat shredded wheat: the clockwise order of the compass points (North, East, South, West).

Richard of York gave battle in vain: the order of colours in a rainbow.

Another popular technique is creating a memory palace. This involves developing a mental image of a real or imaginary location, often a house or palace, that has vivid and distinct locations throughout it.

When trying to memorise a list of words, images, or facts, each one is associated with one of the locations in a vivid or meaningful way. Visualising the memory palace then acts as a prompt to remember the new material.

 

Memory Prompts During Learning

Having an awareness of memory prompts and incorporating them into the learning process will make it easier to recall the newly learnt information at a later date and slow down the steep decline in memory.

Linking new information to prior learning is one of the most effective ways to achieve this; incorporating the new material into a pre-existing schema will allow it to benefit from all of the existing memory prompts and cues that are already in place.

Alternatively, consider using physical cues or the environment as a memory prompt. For example, writing key terms in a different colour pen, in capital letters, or in a certain position on a piece of paper can act as a memory prompt during subsequent recall attempts.

It can also help to learn new material in the same, or similar, environment to where it will need to be recalled; revising in an exam hall in silence would be more effective than revising in a bedroom with music.

 

Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve and retrieval
Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve and retrieval

Implications for Teachers

Understanding how students forget can allow teachers to make changes to their practice to challenge the Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve and increase their students’ memory retention. The following approaches are recommended to teachers in response to Ebbinghaus’ research:

1. Regular Retrieval Practice

Repeated exposure to information strengthens memory retention and flattens the forgetting curve. Asking students to retrieve information forces them to revisit the information, even if they recall it incorrectly, due to the corrective feedback that they receive.

The first retrieval practice should be soon after the new material has been learnt, preferably the following day or in the next lesson. The period of time between each subsequent retrieval should be longer than the previous one. Using spaced intervals are also recommended in Rosenshine’s principles of instruction:

  • Begin each lesson with a review of previous learning
  • Have weekly and monthly reviews of previous learning

It is better to interleave two or more topics together during retrieval practice; this allows the material to be revisited more often and spaced practice to be spread out over time.

2. Review Schemes of Work

Schemes of work should be reviewed to ensure they include regular opportunities for spaced retrieval practice and that topics are arranged by increasing level of difficulty to build on prior learning.

Curriculum plans should be designed to promote mastery by using scaffolding and dividing complex information into smaller and more manageable chunks.

By reducing the volume of new material that students need to learn in each part of the lesson, they are more likely to encode it effectively into their long-term memories.

Lesson plans should make the links to prior learning explicit to students as this will help them assimilate the new information into a pre-existing schema.

It is also helpful to list the keywords and related keywords for each topic as this will help students to reorganise the new information, provide more cues to aid recall, and allow them to make links between related topics.

Promote Metacognition

Encourage students to reflect on what they have learned, but also how they learned it. This will help students to understand which strategies are most effective at improving memory retention.

This is particularly important after a test or assessment, and part of the teacher’s feedback should be focussed on the effectiveness of the revision strategies and processes that the student used.

 

Hermann Ebbinghaus
Hermann Ebbinghaus

Implications for Students 

Being able to accurately recall information gives students a distinct advantage in our current educational system and is a precursor to being able to effectively manipulate and evaluate that information.

Practising active recall strategies and using memory-enhancing techniques as often as possible will challenge the decline in memory that occurs in the absence of retrieval attempts and memory retention strategies.

Active recall requires students to access information from their long-term memory in the absence of any memory cues or prompts.

A brain dump is one of the most simple and effective ways to achieve this. It involves writing down everything the student can remember about a given topic within a specified time frame.

There are no restrictions or demands about what information can be recalled, which means that students will often also benefit from hearing what their peers have been able to recall. Answering practice questions, defining or generating a list of keywords, or completing an assessment are other useful examples of active recall.

Activities that involve passive recall are much less effective at improving memory. These include:

  • Re-reading notes
  • Highlighting text
  • Answering multiple-choice questions

Summarising a page of text or using flashcards can be classified as being either active or passive recall depending on how each task is approached.

Summarising a page of text is a passive recall activity if the page of text is available throughout the task. However, it becomes an active recall activity if the student reads the text, puts it away, and then writes a summary from memory.

The latter approach should be used for answering practice questions; always read the text and hide it before attempting to answer a question about it.

Using flashcards to aid revision by reading the question and then turning over to ‘confirm’ you know what the answer was involves passive recall at best. However, writing down the answer or answering the question out loud before turning over the card to check the answer would be an example of active recall.

Mnemonic techniques are an effective way to boost memory, especially when it is necessary to remember a list of words in order. Students may also benefit from using dual coding to memorise key words or definitions; this involves pairing the new material with a particularly vivid image to make it more memorable.

 

Understanding Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve
Understanding Ebbinghaus Forgetting Curve

Background Information and Further Reading

Hermann Ebbinghaus was a German psychologist who conducted pioneering research on memory and learning in the late 19th century.

He conducted systematic research on himself by memorising series of nonsense syllables (meaningless combinations of three letters) and counting how many he could remember after varying intervals of time.

He discovered that the majority of forgetting happened in the first few hours and that the rate of forgetting decreased over time. 

This trend was presented using the graphical representation of a forgetting curve.  Ebbinghaus’ work was an example of groundbreaking experimental psychology at the time due to the empirical approach he adopted to the study of memory. 

A disadvantage of his approach is that it oversimplifies the human brain and the nature of memory loss by only testing the memorisation of nonsense syllables.  It was difficult to know whether the results could be applied to more natural learning environments.

Subsequent research has supported the concept of an exponential forgetting curve and the conclusions that can be drawn from Ebbinghaus’ research to challenge the decline in memory have been shown to effectively improve memory in real-life settings. To learn more about these studies and the Ebbinghaus forgetting curve, please use the links below.

  1. Murre, J., & Dros, J. (2015). Replication and Analysis of Ebbinghaus’ Forgetting Curve. PLoS ONE, 10.
  2. Roe, D. G., Kim, S., Choi, Y. Y., Woo, H., Kang, M., Song, Y., Ahn, J., Lee, Y., & Cho, J. (2021). Biologically Plausible Artificial Synaptic Array: Replicating Ebbinghaus’ Memory Curve with Selective Attention. Advanced Materials, 33.
  3. Jaber, M., & Bonney, M. (1996). Production breaks and the learning curve: The forgetting phenomenon. Applied Mathematical Modelling, 20, 162-169. 
  4. Miller, R. R. (2021). Failures of memory and the fate of forgotten memories. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 181.
  5. Hewitt, D., Sprague, K., Yearout, R., Lisnerski, D., & Sparks, C. (1992). The effects of unequal relearning rates on estimating forgetting parameters associated with performance curves. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 10, 217-224.